|
|
|
|
|
by Barbara Black
When the brouhaha over judging in pairs figure-skating broke
out at the Salt Lake Olympics, Associate Professor Bryan Campbell got on
the phone to his research partner, John W. Galbraith, of McGill.
The two economists jokingly agreed that it was too bad theyd done
their study on the statistical probability of nationalist bias in figure-skating
judging back in 1996. If they had just done it, they might have been noticed
by the media.
To their surprise, they got a call from the Globe and Mail. Reporter
Stephen Strauss came upon their paper in The Statistician, one of
the journals of the Royal Statistical Society. He called Professor Galbraith,
and referred to their study in an article published the following Saturday,
Feb. 16.
Intellectual fun
Determining whether the nationality of the judges and the skaters affects
the marks awarded in competition posed a delicious challenge to Campbell
and Galbraith, who turned aside from their normal research preoccupations,
economic forecast evaluation and the study of the term structure of interest
rates, to address the question.
It was done for fun, but for intellectual fun that is, in a serious
and scientific way, Professor Campbell said in an interview. Everyone
has an opinion about these things, but there was no literature of empirical
studies of this subject, and sports provides a wealth of reliable statistics.
They wrote to the Olympic Committee and were duly sent data that covered
the judging results for four editions of Olympic Games, including solo and
pairs competitions, and breakdowns as to the categories of judging and the
nationalities of the performers and skaters.
They applied the binomial principle to assessing the possibility of bias.
Campbell explained this with the example of a coin toss. If you flip a coin
many times, you would expect the incidence of its landing heads or tails
to be roughly even. If you assigned a value of plus-one for heads and minus-one
for tails, you would expect the sum to be zero.
In terms of this study, each panel involves nine judges, so in each case,
they used the median mark that of the fifth, or middle, judge. A
score that was above the median received a 1 and a score below a 1.
They also assigned a plus-one to the judge who was from the same country
as the skater or skaters, and a minus-one to the judges who were not from
the skaters country. Then the value of the score was multiplied by
the value associated with nationality, and all the numbers added up. If
there were no nationalist bias, they should still have ended up with a total
close to zero but they didnt.
What they found was that there seemed to be a strong link between the nationality
of the judges and the skaters they favoured but only among the top-seeded
performers.
We looked at the bottom-third ranking of skaters, and there seemed
to be no link at all in terms of nationality, Campbell said. It
was only the top skaters, the stars, who were affected.
A factor of their study that was overlooked in the Globe and Mail article
was that these results didnt point the finger only at the Russians
or the French; they were as true for the Canadian judges as for the others.
It turns out that we also appear to be unduly impressed by our own skaters.
As for using the data to study the likelihood of collusion among judges
of different nationalities, Campbell says that would be a much more
difficult business, and the results obtained would admit of simpler interpretations
than collusion.
Generally, you would have to have a period in the sample where you
were certain there was no collusion; then a period where you compare the
results to the collusion-free period. Then, even if you found some difference,
other interpretations could be advanced remember, there are nine
judges.
|
|
|